The NHL drafts the wrong players due to birthday bias
A hockey player’s birthday strongly biases how professional teams assess his talent, according to a new study by Grand Valley State University researchers.
The research, led by Robert Deaner, associate professor of psychology at Grand Valley, shows that, on average, National Hockey League (NHL) draftees born between July and December are much more likely than those born in the first three months of the year to have successful careers. In particular, 34 percent of draftees were born in the last six months of the year, but these individuals played 42 percent of the games and scored 44 percent of the points accumulated by those in the study. By contrast, those born in the first three months of the year constituted 36 percent of draftees but only played 28 percent of the games and only scored 25 percent of the points.
The study focused on Canadian players because in Canadian youth ice
hockey there is a January 1 cut-off date. This means players born
later in the year would have been consistently younger than their age
group peers.
“There’s no doubt that drafting professional athletes is an
inexact science,” said Deaner. “Plenty of sure-fire first-round picks
fizzle while some late-round picks unexpectedly become stars. But our
results show that, at least since 1980, NHL teams have been
consistently fooled by players’ birthdays or something associated with
them. They greatly underestimate the promise of players born in the
second half of the year, the ones who have always been relatively
younger than their peers. For any given draft slot, relatively younger
players are about twice as likely to be successful. So if teams really
wanted to win, they should have drafted more of the relatively younger players.”
BACKGROUND AND SIGNIFICANCE
Previous studies have
demonstrated relative age effects (RAEs), which occur when those who
are relatively older for their age group are more likely to succeed.
For example, in elite Canadian youth ice hockey, roughly 40 percent of
players are born in the first three months of the year while only 15
percent are born in the last three months. Although RAEs are well
established in many sports and educational settings, their underlying
causes remain unclear. The new study provides the most direct evidence
yet that selection bias is a crucial cause of RAEs. Selection bias
means that evaluators, such as teachers and coaches, grant fewer
opportunities to relatively younger individuals than is warranted by
their talent.
“There are many possible causes of RAEs,” said Deaner. “For
instance, a youth coach may mainly select relatively older players
because those players’ greater size means they are actually more
likely to help the team. Researchers believe, however, that selection
bias is also a big cause of RAEs, but there has never been a direct
test of selection bias. We could make this test because we had a good
measure of perceived talent, the order or slot in which each player
was drafted. And we had good measures of realized talent, how long
they were able to stay in the NHL and how many points they scored
there. Because relatively younger players consistently performed
better than would be expected based on their draft slots, we’ve shown
selection bias.”
The researchers admit that they don’t fully understand the
selection bias they discovered. “We don’t know yet why the evaluations
of NHL teams are biased, but there are several ways it could work.
Because being many months older than one’s peers can be a big
advantage as a child or early teen, the relatively older players might
be more likely to be on the most elite junior teams when they are 17
or 18, and scouts might be swayed by that,” said Deaner. “Another
possibility, suggested by educational studies, is an ‘underdog’
effect. This would involve relatively younger individuals developing
better work habits so that they improve more in adulthood.”
The authors believe their pro hockey results have implications
for education. Deaner noted: “We have to be careful about assuming too
much because a teacher deciding which children should be tracked into
advanced classes is a much different situation than hockey teams
assessing which adults are likely to develop into NHL stars. But, for
many reasons, one would think that NHL teams should be less biased
than educators. First, NHL teams are evaluating adults not children,
meaning that relative age differences are proportionally smaller.
Second, NHL teams are aware of RAEs, but educators may not be. Third,
NHL teams have vast resources to evaluate individuals while educators
do not. Fourth, NHL teams pay a steep price for poor evaluation
whereas educators may not. So overall, in many situations, evaluations
of ability may be greatly colored by an individual’s relative age.
This may even happen when the teachers and coaches know about RAEs.”
Co-authors of the study were Aaron Lowen of Grand Valley State
University and Steven Cobley of the University of Sydney.
For more information, contact Robert Deaner at [email protected].
Subscribe
Sign up and receive the latest Grand Valley headlines delivered to your email inbox each morning.